A Whole Language
Martin Kozloff
September 12, 2002
The founders of whole language (e.g., Kenneth Goodman) asserted in the late 1960's that their so-called revolutionary approach to reading was a "scientific" alternative to then existing approaches; e.g., instruction stressing essential skills such as decoding words using knowledge of sound-symbol correspondence. However, early papers that laid the groundwork for the whole language cult movement reveal that whole language does not at all rest on a scientific way of thinking. In fact, the revolutionary whole language conception of reading as a "psycholinguistic guessing game" is a bizarre fantasy--a fantasy that managed to catch on (and make many thousands of children illiterate) because students in schools of education naively trusted their "literacy" professors--who were more interested in getting tenure, making a reputation, and selling themselves as innovators and self-inflating champions of social justice than they were at making sure new teachers (1) are guided by scientific research (which does not support whole language) and (2) know exactly how to teach reading effectively. In some fields (medicine, law, engineering) this combination of self-aggrandizement, immorality, and ineptitude is called malpractice, fraud, and criminal negligence. In education, it is called "philosophical differences" and "academic freedom." Apparently, school children and new teachers are supposed to pay for the academic freedom of education professors.
See http://people.uncw.edu/kozloffm/goodman.html for an examination of Goodman's early paper.
Following are quotations from leaders in whole language--along with
commentary. Many quotations have been supplied by Dr. Kerry Hempenstall,
whose excellent publications can be found at http://www.rmit.edu.au/departments/ps/staffpgs/hempens.htm.
Without exception, the quotations assert ideas that are false, contrary
to scientific research, and pernicious in their destructive effects on
children. The quotations show a relentless (and a near hysterical) rejection
of the commonsense (and research-supported) ideas that (1) children generally
need a lot of help learning hard skills, (2) the job of teachers is to
teach, and (3) with respect to reading, most children need to be taught
systematically and directly which sounds go with which letters, so that
children can accurately and easily "decode" words rather than simply guess
at them. Yet, the consistency with which whole languagists reject
common sense and decades of experimental research about the importance
of explicit and systematic (rather than occasional and poorly designed)
"phonics" instruction" is instructive. It reveals the duplicity
in
recent claims that whole language is "balanced" instruction. In fact,
the word "balance" is a rhetorical device used to make it appear as if
whole language were consistent with the preponderance of scientific research,
is not harmful to children, and is not an insular cult. [See
Louisa Cook Moats on "The illusion of balanced reading instruction," at
http://www.edexcellence.net/library/wholelang/moats.html.]
Learning to Read is as Easy as Learning a Language. There is No Need for Systematic Instruction. [Wrong.]
"Literacy learning proceeds naturally if the environment supports
young children's experimentation with
print.'' Schickendanz, J. A. (1986). More than the ABC's:
The early stages of reading and writing.
Washington, DC: NAEYC.
[This is one of many trite statements that whole languagists try to
elevate to the level of grand theory to support a mountain of nonsensical
propositions and pernicious "practices." What exactly would experimentation
with print look like? Turning books this way and that? Copying letters?
Making up letters? Are these examples of literacy learning or are
they pre-literacy play that, without instruction, leads straight
to illiteracy?]
"Children must develop reading strategies by and for themselves."
(p.178) Weaver, C. (1988). Reading
process and practice. Exeter, NH: Heinemann.
[This is the basic constructivist mantra about "learners" discovering
knowledge on their own. Advocates of this notion would never allow
physicians to discover brain surgery strategies by operating on advocates'
children. They would never dive out of an airplane in order to discover
the strategy for opening a parachute. They would never toss their
children into a rip current to allow their children to discover the strategy
for not drowning. But somehow it is fine to let other people's children
discover how to read--which, in the long run, means to discover what life
is like when you are illiterate.]
"All proficient readers have acquired an
implicit knowledge of how to read, but this knowledge has been developed
through the practice of reading, not through anything that is taught at school."
Smith, F. (1973). Psychology and reading. New
York: Holt, Rinehart & Winston.
[It is hard to tell if this is to be taken seriously. How exactly
does a person (who does not know how to read) learn to read by reading?
This would appear to be a logical impossibility. Yet, making instructional
claims that are logically absurd is nothing new in whole language.
Besides, does anyone suggest that children learn to read by, for example,
dancing or by making sock puppets? And how is "the practice of reading"
not something taught in school?]
"When language (oral or written) is an integral part of functioning
of a community and is used around and
with neophytes, it is learned 'incidentally.'" Artwergen,
B., Edelsky, C. & Flores, B. (1987). Whole language:
What's new? Reading Teacher 41, 144-154.
[This is an example of airy whole language twaddle that barely rises
to the level of a wish passed off as if it were a universal law of
anthropology. Of course it is true for some children. But without
systematic instruction, many children remain illiterate. What sort of morality
allows writers to make such hyperbolic claims that are akin to sales pitches
at medicine shows?]
"Learning is continuous, spontaneous, and effortless, requiring
no particular attention, conscious
motivation, or specific reinforcement." (p. 432) Smith, F. (1992).
Learning to read: The never-ending debate. Phi
Delta Kappan, 74, 432-441.
[This statement is true if you are talking about sucking a lollipop.
But as soon as you try to walk, ride a bike, read, learn a second language,
or calculate the second derivative, you find that learning is nothing like
Smith's preposterous statement.]
"Saying that we are determined to teach every child to read does
not mean that we will teach every child to read.'' (p.441) Smith, F.
(1992). Learning to read: the never-ending debate. Phi Delta Kappan,
74, 432-441.
[Too bad that this caveat is buried in whole language writing.
If it were the first line, any rational teacher would say "No, thanks,
I believe I'll pass," and any right-thinking parent would call a lawyer.]
"The best we can do ... is ... to ensure that, if not every child
lives up to our hopes, there is a minimum of guilt and anguish on the part
of teachers, students, and parents." (p.441) Smith, F. (1992).
Learning to read: the never-ending debate. Phi Delta Kappan, 74,
432-441.
[Does this even require comment? The best the whole
languagist can do is to ensure that he or she does not feel badly about
making children illiterate?! Nothing about examining whole language
to see how it damages children and then scrapping it. Just ensure
that you feel no anguish. Can you imagine this kind of talk
in medicine? A doctor says, "Well, I do kill half my patients, but
I manage not to feel too much guilt and anguish. You might say I am self-actualizing."]
"Methods can never ensure that children learn to read. .... It is
the relationships that exist within the
classroom that matter. ... Tests are not required to find out whether
children are learning." (p.440) Smith, F.
(1992). Learning to read: The never-ending debate. Phi Delta Kappan,
74, 432-441.
[This appears to be the basis for the claim that no method is needed
to teach reading. And no method is exactly what whole language provides--which
is why so many new teachers fresh from schools of education say, "I have
no idea how to teach kids to read. I know how to write a literacy
philosophy, but not how to teach." ]
"The child is already programmed to learn to read." Smith, F.
(1973).
Psychology and reading. New York:
Holt, Rinehart & Winston.
[Another bit of sediment from the whole language bilge bucket.
This appeal to "naturalism" is apparently supposed to obviate the need
to teach children to read because, being programmed to read, they will
teach themselves. How then account for illiteracy? It must
be the kids' fault--or maybe their parents' fault.]
"Children can develop and use an intuitive knowledge of letter-sound
correspondences [without] any
phonics instruction [or] without deliberate instruction from adults."
(p. 86) Weaver, C. (1980).
Psycholinguistics and reading. Cambridge, MA: Winthrop.
[What exactly would intuitive knowledge of letter-sound
correspondence be? Does m look like it says /m/? How does
a person intuit what the words stupid idiot sound like? And what does it
mean to say that children "can develop and use..."? The question
is How many ever do? But whole languagists never answer this
question, because it would show that whole language does not work anything
like as well as is claimed. In other words, No data, no responsibility,
no blame = business (tenure, consulting gigs, publications, control over
education schools) as usual.]
"We cannot teach another person directly; we can only facilitate
his learning." Rogers, C. (1961). On
becoming a person. Boston: Houghton Mifflin.
[No doubt Rogers had a lot of data to support this delusional statement.
Apparently Rogers was not up to the idea that a teacher can facilitate
learning by teaching some things directly. As with latter-day whole
languagists, Rogers may have been more comfortable with binary oppositions.
Either x or y but not both." This is called simpleminded.]
Skilled Readers Do Not Decode Words (See and Read the
Letters).
They Guess, Using Contextual Cues. [Wrong]
"Phonics is incompatible with a whole language perspective on reading
and therefore is rejected."
Watson, D. (1989). Defining & describing whole language. Elementary
School Journal, 90, 129-142.
[This is a fine example of whole language dogma. You don't reject something
because the data say it does not work. You reject something because
is is incompatible with a set of beliefs (impervious to criticism) that
are organized into a cult. And yet whole languagists try to call what they
do science. So did the architects of the soviet system of economics.]
"Reading without guessing is not reading at all." Smith,
F. (1973).
Psychology and reading. New York: Holt,
Rinehart & Winston.
[I would ask the reader if he or she is guessing at the words he or
she is reading now--or is it feeding how, or bleeding cow, or dreading
wow? Smith's arrogant assertion is a ploy designed to bolster the
injunction against teaching children to decode words through knowledge
of letter-sound correspondence. We wonder just how much guessing
is a child supposed to do before it is called reading? "Look at Cherie.
She is guessing at every single word. She's a real
reader. But look at Debra. No guessing at all.
She knows exactly what every words says. That's not reading!"
So stupid.]
"Proficient readers seem unconsciously to use initial letters plus
prior knowledge and context to predict
what a word might be, before focusing on more of the word or the
following context to confirm or correct."
Weaver, C. (Phonics in whole language classrooms) at: http://kidsource.com/kidsource/content2/Phonics.html
[Does Weaver seriously want to construct an approach to reading instruction
based on what children "seem" to do? Is the whole languagist really capable
of mind-reading as well--somehow knowing whether children focus only on
the first letter or rapidly scan the whole word and say it fast? If whole
languagists really can read children's minds--which may be true because
they say they do not need test data to tell if children can read--then
they ought to offer additional services to schools--services such as channeling
Carl Rogers. The point is, How does anyone know what other people
are doing unconsciously?]
The student: Attends to the meaning of what is read rather
than focusing on figuring out words. Uses
context, pictures, syntax, and structural analysis clues to predict
meaning of unknown words. Uses fix-it
strategies (predicts, uses pictorial cues,asks a friend, skips the
word, substitutes another meaningful word).
Oklahoma State Department of Education (1992). Reading learner outcomes.
In the Oklahoma State Competencies,
Grade One, pp.15-22. [Online]. Available: http://www.ourcivilisation.com/dumb/dumb3.htm
[The good people at the Oklahoma State D.O.E. attempt to provide a
rational for not teaching students to decode words by using knowledge of
sound-symbol correspondence. The glaring logical fault, however--glaring
to anyone except the good people at the Oklahoma State D.O.E.--is that
the statement applies to poor readers. It is nice to
know that the good people at the Oklahoma State D.O.E. want teachers to
help children to use the inefficient, error-filled, frustrating, and basically
inept methods used by struggling readers; i.e., children who were earlier
mistaught by whole language. This is an example of the reproduction
of illiteracy from one generation to the next--as the past generation's
reading incompetence is reinterpreted as competence, and is then
presented to teachers as a model of how to teach. This also helps to
ensure a steady supply of struggling first graders for Reading Recovery.
Let us recall that Orwell's 1984 referred to the time that whole
language--and whole language newspeak--came to power.]
"It is easier for a reader to remember the unique appearance and
pronunciation of a whole word like
'photograph' than to remember the unique pronunciations of meaningless
syllables and spelling units"
(p.146) Smith, F. (1985). Reading without nonsense: Making
sense of reading. New York: Teachers College
Press.
[Smith must be insensitive to irony. Surely he is not referring
to his own book when he writes about reading without nonsense. Of
course it is easier to remember one word by sight than to
learn the sounds that go with each letter. What Smith neglects to
tell the reader is that if a child memorizes ten words, the child can read
only
ten
words, but if the child learns the sounds of ten letters, the child will
be able to read 350 three-sound words, 4,320 four-sound words, and 21,650
five-sound words. Moreover, if the child merely memorizes (but cannot
sound out) "photograph," what is the child likely to "read" when the child
bumps into "phosphate," "phonograph," and "phony ass?"]
"One word in five can be completely eliminated from most English
texts with scarcely any effect on its
overall comprehensibility." (p.79) Smith, F. (1973). Psychology
and reading. New York: Holt, Rinehart &
Winston.
[And the implication is....? Therefore, let's get rid of 20 percent
of the words in Shakespeare? Or, let's try that with Smith's own
statement and see if it has scarcely any effect on its comprehensibility.
"One can be completely eliminated from most with any effect on its overall
comprehensibility." Yes, that means the same thing.]
Phonics Instruction is Not Needed. In fact, it is Bad. [Wrong]
"Sounding out a word is a cumbersome, time-consuming, and unnecessary
activity. By using context, we
can identify words with only minimal attention to grapho/phonemic
cues. The message then seems clear: we
should help children learn to use context first." Weaver, C.
(1988). Reading process & practice: From
socio-psycholinguistics to whole language. Portsmouth, NH: Heinemann.
[Teachers who wish to ensure that most of their students remain illiterate
should follow Weaver's orders. By this statement, Weaver shows clearly
that the whole language claim of balanced reading is simply a lie.
Note that Weaver presents no data to support her bogus claim about sounding
out words--there are no such data--but from her groundless and merely dogmatic
statement she draws a "message." This nonrational process of finding
messages in a mess of verbiage is akin to predicting the future from sheep
guts. If it is a message, it is not from this or any other known
world. But, again, we see that whole languagists have special powers.]
"Matching letters with sounds is a flat-earth view of the world,
one that rejects modern science about
reading." (p. 371) Goodman, K. S. (1986). What's
whole in whole language. Richmond Hill, Ontario: Scholastic.
[Goodman's statement wins the prize for irony and self-delusion--a
sort of biathlon. Whole language is to modern science as throwing
trash at a wall is to art, as Da Da is to poetry, as pounding a piano with
a salami is to music. But let us remember that Goodman was working
for a revolution in reading instruction--a revolution (as with other revolutions)
that requires followers to become maximally stupid so that they do not
detect the essential madness in their leaders' flatulent eructations.
"The man must be a prophet! He sounds completely insane!"]
"Reading by 'phonics' is demonstrably impossible (ask any computer)."
Smith,
J. (1986). Essays into literacy. Exeter, NH: Heinemann.
[With this line, Smith clearly demonstrates that he may know as little
about computers as he does about reading--a two-for-one deal. What is in
fact impossible to demonstrate is anything but sight reading and guessing
in kids who know nothing about phonics--i.e., which sounds go with which
letters. Throwing in the cute comment about computers may be Smith's
way of diverting attention from the fact that good readers rapidly decode
words on the basis of phonics knowledge.]
"In my view, reading is not a matter of decoding letters to sound
but of bringing meaning to print." Smith, J. (1986). Essays into
literacy. Exeter, NH: Heinemann.
[This is a common ploy in whole language: create a false dichotomy
that makes one "side" look ridiculous (teaching decoding) and therefore
the other side (teaching reading for meaning--allegedly whole language)
looks like a star. However, what Smith (no doubt accidentally) fails
to say is that: (1) Good teachers teach both decoding and
reading for meaning; and (2) It is demonstrably impossible to "bring meaning
to print" unless you already know how to read what the words say.
Mr. Smith, as with so many whole language gurus and their followers, is
apparently unaware of the logical absurdities in their "philosophy."]
"English is spelled so unpredictably that there is no way of predicting
when a particular spelling
correspondence applies" (p. 53) Smith, F. (1985). Reading.
New
York: Cambridge University Press.
[How often do readers predict what words say? What would
that even look like? "I predict that w h e n says /when/." And then the
reader waits around for the word on the page to start making sounds to
see if the prediction is borne out? So stupid it makes your nose
run. Readers simply read--correctly (i.e., the way other competent
readers read)--or incorrectly. They read incorrectly when they don't
know what sounds go with what letters--i.e., when they were mistaught by
whole language.]
"Phonics, which means teaching a set of spelling to sound correspondence
rules that permit the decoding of
written language into speech, just does not work." Smith,
F. (1985).
Reading without nonsense (2nd. Ed). New
York: Teachers College Press.
[When you read enough whole language guff, you begin to induce a rule
about what they are up to. The rule is something like, "Say the most
outrageous things--that are the absolute opposite of obvious fact--and
you can be sure that you will mystify your audience."... "He must be in
contact with Higher Powers, because he sounds utterly demented."
Notice the smug self-confidence that oozes from the phrase, "just
does not work," as if there were any data to back it up--and of course
there are
no data.]
"Carefully controlled vocabulary and decontextualised phonics instruction
are incompatible with meaningful authentic texts." Goodman, K.
S. (1989). Whole language research: Foundations and development. The
Elementary School Journal, 90, 208-221.
[This is a type of illogic called "straw man." Obviously,
if you teach only letter-sound correspondence it will become
pretty meaningless. Just as it would become pretty meaningless if
you only taught the atomic weights of the elements and never taught students
to use this knowledge in doing experiments. But of course any teacher
with a minimum of brain tissue would be using phonics instruction
(a little each day) as a means to an end--namely, having students
accurately read and understand text. But whole language ideologues have
to create a straw man (endless phonics instruction) or else they
have no place to stand as self-created revolutionaries and adversaries
of explicit phonics instruction..]
"To the fluent reader the alphabetic principle is completely irrelevant.
He identifies every word (if he
identifies words at all) as an ideogram." (p.124) Smith,
F. (1973). Psycholinguistics and reading. New York:
Holt, Rhinehart, & Winston.
[Apparently, Smith made a typo at an important spot. He must
have meant to write, "To the struggling reader mistaught with whole language,
the alphabetic principle..." Because besides students who do not
know what sounds letters make, the only other readers on this planet who
memorize words from the shape of words are readers of Chinese, Japanese,
and a few other languages that use pictures for words. But readers
of alphabetic languages--not yet damaged by whole language nonsense--generally
use the alphabet. That's pretty much what it's for.]
"We might offer students some phonics hints at an appropriate moment
when they are writing and aren't
sure how to spell something." Newman, J.M., & Church, S.M.
(1991). Myths of whole language. The Reading
Teacher, 44, 20-26.
[It's probably a great comfort to struggling readers to know that their
"facilitators" are going to give a few hints (so that students can continue
to struggle and guess) when their facilitators could just as easily have
said, "That sound is sss." But no, to the whole language cult diehard,
actual information would thwart the struggling reader's path to developmentally
appropriate illiteracy. Moreover, to give sound information, rather
than hints, makes the whole language teacher a mere teacher, rather than
some kind of Rogerian demi-god therapist and artiste who occasionally deigns
to give suffering clients a hint or two. The most highly developed
skill of the whole language con artist is disguising his or her essentially
immoral "project" behind a curtain of high sounding bunk.]
"Carefully controlled vocabulary and decontextualised phonics instruction
are incompatible with meaningful authentic texts." Goodman, K.
S. (1989). Whole language is whole: A response to Heymsfeld. Educational
Leadership, 69-70.
[Apparently, Goodman has difficulty separating instruction from
application of skill. Of course if a teacher is working on "phonics"
("This sound is /m/") it would be incompatible with reading authentic texts—or
any texts--just as making a sandwich and at the same time eating the sandwich
are incompatible. Mr. Goodman cannot admit that instruction (learning
to read) is separable from application (reading) because whole language
is based on the nutty idea that children learn to read without explicit
instruction in elementary reading skills. In whole language fantasy land,
children learn to read while they are reading—which makes as much
sense (and is about as immoral) as saying that surgeons will learn to do
surgery while they are operating on your children.]
"The worst readers are those who try to sound out unfamiliar words
according to the rules of phonics." (p.438) Smith, F. (1992). Learning
to read: the never-ending debate. Phi Delta Kappan, 74, 432-441.
[Note that Mr. Smith presents no data for this absurd statement.
In fact, readers who guess at words are the worst readers—indeed, they
are not even reading.]
"Early in our miscue research, we concluded…That a story is easier
to read than a page, a page easier to read than a paragraph, a paragraph
easier than a sentence, a sentence easier than a word, and a word easier
than a letter. Our research continues to support this conclusion and we
believe it to be true…" Goodman, K. & Goodman, Y. (1981). Twenty
questions about teaching language. Educational Leadership, 38, 437-442.
[These lines have a nice rhythm--and they make sense if you have lost
your mind. The sane person wants to know how a child who cannot easily
read a letter is able easily to read a word (which consists entirely of
letters); how a child who cannot easily read a word is able easily to read
a sentence (which consists entirely of words); how a child who cannot easily
read a sentence is able easily to read a paragraph (which consists entirely
of sentences); and how a child who cannot easily read a paragraph is able
easily to read a story. One wonders what kind of "research" would
support the Goodmans' backwards-land belief. Must be from another
world.]
Phonemic Awareness and the Alphabetic Principle are Insignificant. [Wrong]
“The art of becoming a fluent reader lies in learning to rely less
and less on information from the eyes.” Smith, F. (1975). Comprehension
& learning: A conceptual framework for teachers. New York: Richard
C. Owen.
[One wonders where children with sight get their information when reading,
if not from what they see on the page. Maybe—generalizing from Goodman's
fantasy about reading being a guessing game—children stare into space and
imagine what is on the page. Or maybe they listen real hard to what
the letters on the page are saying.]
"Accuracy, correctly naming or identifying each word or word part
in a graphic sequence, is not necessary for effective reading since the
reader can get the meaning without accurate word identification. Furthermore,
readers who strive for accuracy are likely to be inefficient" (p.826)
Goodman, K. S. (1974, Sept). Effective teachers of reading know language
and children. Elementary English, 51, 823-828.
[This is another example of whole languagists concocting utter rubbish
in order to sell their failed methods. In fact, readers who are taught—by
whole language—to guess at words are inefficient readers—indeed, they are
disabled
readers--because they are often wrong. They mistake lion and
lying, this and these, the and there, car and can, etc. Obviously,
accurate
reading is necessary for getting the meaning. "The car
is fast" does not mean the same thing as "The can is fat." And "Caution.
Toxic fumes" does not mean the same thing as "Caution. Toxic
tunes." And the error is costly.]
"It has become crystal clear to me--and it has taken about ten years
to come to this understanding--that children learn phonics best after they
can already read. I am convinced that the reason our good readers
are good at phonics is that in their being able to read they can intuitively
make sense of phonics" (p. 44) Routman, R. (1994). Invitations.
Portsmouth,
NH : Heinemann.
[Routman makes the same logical error as Goodman and Smith. Students
are alleged to learn phonics—that is, which sounds are made by which letters—after
they
have learned to read. This is logically impossible, because reading
means (among other things) saying the sounds made by the letters.
What would "reading" look like if a child did not know that m says /m/
and a says /a/? Is that what any sane person calls reading?]
“Breaking whole language into bite-size, abstract little pieces,
words, syllables, and isolated sounds makes learning to read more difficult.”
Goodman,
K.S. (1986). What's whole in whole language. Richmond Hill, Ontario:
Scholastic.
[This is exactly the opposite of what serious research—for
about 100 years—says. It is easier to ride a bike if you first
learn
how to move your legs and use your hands to hold on. It is easier
to learn to swim if you first learn how to kick and paddle and breathe.
It is harder to learn to skydive if you do not know the elementary
skills. In fact, you will die before you learn--just as many kids
who get whole language become illiterate because they don't know the elementary
skills of reading.]
Scientific (Controlled, Quantitative) Research and Accountability Are Unnecessary. [Wrong]
"It seems futile to try to demonstrate superiority of one teaching
method over another by empirical research." (p.220) Weaver, C. (1988).
Reading:
Progress and practice. Portsmouth, NJ: Heinemann.
[It is not at all futile. It has been done many times, and whole
language is usually shown to be far inferior to explicit instruction that
focuses first on the elements of reading. However, if Weaver had
gotten enough people to believe it is futile, then no one would know how
bad whole language is.]
"Only one kind of research has anything useful to say about literacy,
and that is ethnographic or naturalistic research." (p. 356) Smith,
K. (1989). Overselling literacy. Phi Delta Kappan, 70, 353-359.
[This is surely a self-serving statement—because ethnographic research
(uncontrolled and not subject to reliability checks)—can be made to provide
data for any conclusions you want. Only quantitative, experimental
research can pit one approach against another, and tell how much
children learned and how fast they learned it. But whole languagists
do not want these kinds of data available because these data show that
whole language is inferior to explicit instruction.]
"(Teachers are) wise to the often tortuous attempts of educational,
psychological, and cognitive researchers to cloak themselves in the sometimes
ill-fitting garb of 'science.'" Zemelman, S., Daniels, H., &
Bizar, M. (1999, March). Sixty years of reading research -- But who's listening?
Phi Delta Kappan. [Online.] Available: http://www.pdkintl.org/kappan/kzem9903.htm
[In fact, whole languagists are the ones who clothe their nonsensical
"theories" and bizarre instructional methods in the garb of science—which,
to them, means a few field notes ("ethnographic research"). Of course
they do not like serious research—just as new age "healers" do not like
experimental research—because it makes it clear to consumers that they
are frauds.]
"In my inaugural [Convention] address I called for a greater separation
between school and state and the emancipation of education from the arbitrariness
of political pressures. I advanced the idea that schools, like religion
and the press, needed the protection of something like a Constitutional
amendment to keep education free of interference in matters of materials,
methods, and curriculum from the winds of political change and the passing
hysterias of public opinion." (NCTE president, Sheridan Blau) National
Council of Teachers of English. (1999). Elementary school practices. [On-Line].
Available at http://ncte.org
[Blau fails to point out is that the only reason the "state" got into
the education business is to protect the public from fads—such as
whole language—that have injured so many children. This is no different
from the state protecting citizens from poison passed off as medicine.]
Don't Correct Errors. Don't Ensure Mastery of Fundamentals. [Wrong]
"The first alternative and preference is - to skip over the puzzling
word. The second alternative is to guess what the unknown word might
be. And the final and least preferred alternative is to sound the word
out. Phonics, in other words, comes last." Smith, F. (1999).
Why systematic phonics and phonemic awareness instruction constitute an
educational hazard. Language Arts, 77, 150-155.
[Let's try this in medicine. The first alternative—when you make
an error--is to skip over it. The second alternative is to guess
at what you should do. And the last and least preferred alternative
is to do the procedure carefully step by step. In other words, Smith
tells teachers exactly how to ensure that children make the most errors
possible; never learn that they are errors (just skip over them);
and only as a last resort, do it right—say the sounds of the letters.
For whole language, up is down, false is true, and stupidity is wisdom.
Note that there was not one shred of evidence—and there is still not one
shred of evidence—to support Smith's assertion that sounding out words
is the least preferred and should be the last method used. In fact,
the research clearly says that sounding out unfamiliar words is the
most preferred thing to do.]
"Good spelling is merely a convenience. … There are some people
like secretaries, who need to be accurate, but usually even they can use
a word processor with a good spelling check." Gentry, J.R. (1987).
Spel
. . . is a four-letter word. Portsmouth: Heinemann.
[Yes, good spelling is merely a convenience if you want society to
become dumber and dumber and dumber. Why learn math; calculators
can do it? Why read history books; just get the Cliff Notes?
It is amazing how far into fantasy land whole languagists go to preserve
their idiotic approach.]
In the Absence of Logic, Scientific Evidence, and Moral Responsibility, Attack Your Critics.
" … the interlocking directorate of the right-wing back-to-basics
movement: John Saxon, Chester Finn, William Bennett, Diane Ravitch, Jeanne
Chall, Charles Sykes." Zemelman, S., Daniels, H., & Bizar, M. (1999,
March). Sixty years of reading research -- But who's listening? Phi Delta
Kappan. [Online.]
Available: http://www.pdkintl.org/kappan/kzem9903.htm
[This is a common form of argument in whole language. Simply
insult critics—but provide no evidence to support the insult. This
is like charlatan "healers" who attack real physicians.]
"It (direct instruction) is a scripted pedagogy for producing compliant,
conformist, competitive students and adults." Coles, G. (1998, Dec.
2). No end to the reading wars. Education Week. [Online]. Available:
http://www.edweek.org/ew/vol-18/14coles.h18.
[This is typical of whole language reasoning and discussion.
They present no data at all to support their attacks, and rely only on
evocative words. But then, that is basically all there is to whole
language, anyway. See
http://people.uncw.edu/kozloffm/coles.html]