The evolution of creativity

Creativity is often considered to be one of the defining hallmarks of modern human behaviour. Whilst there are considerable cultural differences in the definition and expression of creativity, there is evidence from ethnography, archaeology, and ethology that creative products such as painting, dance, humour, and story telling are evolutionary old and universal behaviours (Darwin, 1901; Dissanayake, 1988, 1992; McBrearty & Brooks, 2000; Miller, 1998; Mithen, 1996; Power, 1999). While a universal trait may have arisen as a by-product or neutral trait, universality may suggest that the trait under review was selected for through evolutionary mechanisms. Thus universality makes an evolutionary approach relevant.

Cognitive prerequisites

There have been a number of theories as to the cognitive prerequisites necessary for creativity to have evolved. Carruthers (2002) considers childhood pretence as fundamental to the development of adult creative abilities. He proposes that there would have been two cognitive elements required during an episode of pretence, a supposition-generator, to allow humans to imagine possibilities that had not previously occurred, and a 'possible worlds box' which acted as a memory system to allow these suppositions and the thoughts around these to be stored.

Mithen (1998) also discusses the cognitive prerequisites required for human creativity and proposes that these are a theory of mind (TOM), language, and a complex material culture leading to an extended mind. These cognitive abilities then came together to enable the emergence of cognitive fluidity, which facilitates the production of creative thought by allowing the mind to combine concepts across the social, technical and natural history domains

Another possible cognitive prerequisite for creativity is an aesthetic appreciation (Dissanayake, 1988; 1992; Orians, 2001; Orians & Heerwagen, 1992; Thornhill, 1998), which would have been a much earlier precursor for the evolution of creative abilities. The ability to find something beautiful or ugly can produce powerful positive or negative emotions respectively. These emotions can then trigger appropriate behaviours in response to the object. In fact, Orians (2001:3) defines beauty as

'…the product of interactions between traits of objects and the human nervous system that evolved so that objects we consider beautiful have properties that result in improved performance in some aspect of living if we respond positively to them.'

The emergence of creative abilities

In an attempt to explain the reason why the emergence of anatomically modern Homo sapiens at approximately 150,000 years ago and the appearance of creative activity do not coincide, Carruthers (2002) states that the cognitive prerequisites for creativity were present when anatomically modern humans first appeared, at approximately 150,000 years ago, but that between this time and the creative explosion, at around 30 to 40, 000 years ago, there was selection for a predisposition for children to engage in frequent pretend play.

In a similar attempt to understand this time lag, Mithen (1996:222) proposes that cognitive fluidity began to occur with the emergence of modern Homo sapiens and was a gradual process, which was completed between 60,000 and 30,000 years ago when creative behaviour is first observed in the archaeological record.

According to Dissanayake (1992) the activity of 'making special', which she claims is the common behavioural denominator in the arts, arose as early as 300,000 years ago with the use of red ochre or hematite, although an aesthetic sensibility was present much earlier by the time of Homo erectus if not Homo habilis (Dissanayake, 1988). In fact, Dissanayake states that body decoration can be considered as the prototype of the visual arts. In general Neanderthals are thought not to have possessed fully symbolic minds. However, from the viewpoint of this theory Neanderthals would not have needed to be symbolisers to have appreciated specialness. Nevertheless, Dissanayake suggests that it was not until anatomically modern humans became aware of the past and future that there was a need to make things special to deal with the uncertainty that these concepts evoked. Therefore this led to a move to deliberately make things special. However, Neanderthals may not have possessed such understandings of time (Lewis-Williams, 2002) and so whilst able to appreciate specialness may not have considered a need for it which would explain the scarcity of clear archaeological evidence for the Neanderthal manufacture of the arts.

Natural or sexual selection

Carruthers (2002) suggests that creativity is an indicator of intelligence and problem solving ability, and therefore that it would have been selected for through both natural and sexual selection. This is further supported by Orians (2001) and Thornhill (1998) when they claim that what is felt to be beautiful, such as high degrees of symmetry in human faces, is believed to indicate high levels of reproductive and survival fitness whereas what is perceived as ugly suggests reduced reproductive and survival fitness.

Dissanayake (1988; 1992) considers that the arts could have been co-opted by sexual selection to act as fitness indicators but that their original function was to promote the survival of the group and hence the individuals within the group. To do this the arts, such as song, music and dance, were used in rituals to elicit and provide controlled expression of emotions (Dissanayake, 1992).

Darwin (1871/1901) was one of the first to suggest that physical and psychological traits in humans could have evolved through sexual selection. In fact he states that 'higher powers' such as imagination will have evolved partly through sexual selection and partly through natural selection (Darwin, 1901:859-60). However, since Darwin argued that choice came not only from competition but also through female choosiness his ideas on sexual selection were largely ignored for several decades.

Miller & Haselton (2002) support the sexual selection hypothesis for the emergence of creativity. In a study that they conducted poor male artists were found to be more successful in short-term matings than wealthy, less creative men when females were at a point in their menstrual cycle when they had a higher risk of conception. However, creative individuals may also be more successful in long-term matings. For example, Buss (pers. com) supports the long-term strategy when he describes humour, which may be considered a creative product, as a resource acquisition since humour implies high intelligence and this could be employed in the attainment of resources.

Summary

Recently a number of interesting theories on the evolution of creativity have emerged, only some of which have been discussed here. There have also been a number of theories built up around the evolution of specific creative products such as Upper Palaeolithic cave paintings (Lewis-Williams, 2002), and music (Brown, 2000; Dissanayake, 2000; Falk, 2000; Hagen & Bryant, 2002; Miller, 2000). This has opened up opportunities for research to provide empirical support or to refute these hypotheses. Thus, an exciting chapter is opening in the discovery of one of the most fundamental traits that defines us as human, creativity.

References

Brown, S. 2000. The 'Musilanguage' Model of Music Evolution. In N.L. Wallin, B. Merker & S. Brown (eds) The Origins of Music. pp 271-300 Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press

Carruthers, P. 2002. Human Creativity: Its Cognitive Basis, Its evolution, and Its Connections With Childhood Pretence. British Journal For The Philosophy of Science. 53(2)

Darwin, C. 1901. The Descent of Man and Selection in Relation to Sex. 3rd edition. London: John Murray

Dissanayake, E. 1988. What Is Art For? Seattle: University of Washington Press

Dissanayake, E. 1992. Homo Aestheticus. Where art Comes From And Why. New York: The Free Press

Dissanayake, E. 2000. Antecedents of the Temporal Arts in Early Mother-Infant Interaction. In N.L. Wallin, B. Merker & S. Brown (eds) The Origins of Music. pp 389-410 Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press

Falk, D. 2000. Hominid Brain Evolution and the Origins of Music. In N.L. Wallin, B. Merker & S. Brown (eds) The Origins of Music. pp 197-216 Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press

Hagen, E.H. & Bryant, G.A. 2002. Music and Dance as a Coalition Signalling System. Cogprints Database

Lewis-Williams, D. 2002. The Mind In The Cave. Consciousness And The Origin Of Art. London: Thames & Hudson

McBrearty, S. & Brooks, A.S. 2000. The Revolution That Wasn’t: A New Interpretation of the Origin of Modern Human Behaviour. Journal of Human Evolution 39 453-563

Miller, G.F. 1998. How Mate Choice Shaped Human nature: A review of Sexual Selection and Human Evolution. In C. Crawford & D.L. Krebs (eds) Handbook of Evolutionary Psychology. Ideas, issues and Applications. pp. 87-129. New Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates

Miller, G.F. 2000. Evolution of Human Music through Sexual Selection. In N.L. Wallin, B. Merker & S. Brown (eds) The Origins of Music. pp 329-360 Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press

Miller, G.F. & Haselton, M. 2002. Fertile Women Prefer Poor, Creative Men to Wealthy, Uncreative Men as short-Term Sexual Partners: Preliminary Evidence for Ovulatory Cycle Shifts in Attraction to Artistic and entrepreneurial Excellence. Abstract from Human Behavioural and Evolutionary Society Conference.

Mithen, S. 1996. The Prehistory of the Mind. A Search for the Origins of Art, Religion and Science London: Phoenix

Mithen, S. 1998. A Creative Explosion? Theory of Mind, Language and the Disembodied Mind of the Upper Palaeolithic. In S. Mithen (ed) Creativity in Human Evolution and Prehistory. pp. 165-191. UK: Routledge

Orians, G.H. 2001. An Evolutionary Perspective On Aesthetics. Bulletin of Psychology and the Arts 2(1)

Orians, G.H. & Heerwagen, J.H. 1992. Evolved Responses to Landscapes. In J.H. Barkow, L. Cosmides & J. Tooby (eds) The Adapted Mind. Evolutionary Psychology and the Generation of Culture. pp. 555-579. New York: Oxford University Press.

Power, C. 1999. 'Beauty Magic' : The Origins of Art. In R. Dunbar, C. Knight & C. Power (eds) The Evolution of Culture. pp.92-112. Edinburgh: Edinburgh University Press.

Thornhill, R. 1998. Darwinian Aesthetics. In C. Crawford & D.L. Krebs (eds) Handbook of Evolutionary Psychology. Ideas, issues and Applications. pp.543-572. New Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates

© Helen Keenoo, 2004, student at the Open University.

From:  Imaginative Minds: an Interdisciplinary Symposium was a two-day event funded by the British Academy which took place on 30 April and 1 May 2004, convened by Dr Ilona Roth, senior lecturer in psychology at the Open University.